Resolution of front-back ambiguity in spatial hearing by listener and source movement

被引:242
作者
Wightman, FL [1 ]
Kistler, DJ
机构
[1] Univ Wisconsin, Dept Psychol, Madison, WI 53705 USA
[2] Univ Wisconsin, Waisman Ctr, Madison, WI 53705 USA
关键词
D O I
10.1121/1.426899
中图分类号
O42 [声学];
学科分类号
070206 ; 082403 ;
摘要
Normally, the apparent position of a sound source corresponds closely to its actual position. However, in some experimental situations listeners make large errors, such as indicating that a source in the frontal hemifield appears to be in the rear hemifield, or vice versa. These front-back confusions are thought to be a result of the inherent ambiguity of the primary interaural difference cues, interaural time difference (ITD) in particular. A given ITD could have been produced by a sound source anywhere on the so-called "cone of confusion." More than 50 years ago Wallach [J. Exp. Psychol. 27, 339-368 (1940)] argued that small head movements could provide the information necessary to resolve the ambiguity. The direction of the change in ITD that accompanies a head rotation is an unambiguous indicator of the proper hemifield. The experiments reported here are a modern test of Wallach's hypothesis, Listeners indicated the apparent positions of real and virtual sound sources in conditions in which head movements were either restricted or encouraged. The front-back confusions made in the restricted condition nearly disappeared in the condition in which head movements were encouraged. In a second experiment head movements were restricted, but the sound source was moved, either by the experimenter or by the listener. Only when the listener moved the sound source did front-back confusions disappear. The results clearly support Wallach's hypothesis and suggest further that head movements are not required to produce the dynamic cues needed to resolve front-back ambiguity. (C) 1999 Acoustical Society of America. [S0001-4966(99)05005-5].
引用
收藏
页码:2841 / 2853
页数:13
相关论文
共 24 条
[1]   LOCALIZATION OF REAL AND VIRTUAL SOUND SOURCES [J].
BRONKHORST, AW .
JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 1995, 98 (05) :2542-2553
[2]  
CLEVELAND WS, 1985, ELEMENTS GRAPHING DA, P161
[3]  
Durlach N.I., 1992, Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Enviroments, Volume, V1, P251, DOI [10.1162/pres.1992.1.2.251, DOI 10.1162/PRES.1992.1.2.251]
[4]  
GREEN DM, 1988, PROFILE ANAL, P93
[5]   LOCALIZATION OF SOUND IN ROOMS [J].
HARTMANN, WM .
JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 1983, 74 (05) :1380-1391
[6]   A MODEL OF HEAD-RELATED TRANSFER-FUNCTIONS BASED ON PRINCIPAL COMPONENTS-ANALYSIS AND MINIMUM-PHASE RECONSTRUCTION [J].
KISTLER, DJ ;
WIGHTMAN, FL .
JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 1992, 91 (03) :1637-1647
[7]   2-DIMENSIONAL SOUND LOCALIZATION BY HUMAN LISTENERS [J].
MAKOUS, JC ;
MIDDLEBROOKS, JC .
JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 1990, 87 (05) :2188-2200
[8]   ACUITY OF SOUND LOCALIZATION - A TOPOGRAPHY OF AUDITORY SPACE .1. NORMAL HEARING CONDITIONS [J].
OLDFIELD, SR ;
PARKER, SPA .
PERCEPTION, 1984, 13 (05) :581-600
[9]   The contribution of head motion cues to localization of low-pass noise [J].
Perrett, S ;
Noble, W .
PERCEPTION & PSYCHOPHYSICS, 1997, 59 (07) :1018-1026
[10]   The effect of head rotations on vertical plane sound localization [J].
Perrett, S ;
Noble, W .
JOURNAL OF THE ACOUSTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA, 1997, 102 (04) :2325-2332