Can we talk to robots? Ten-month-old infants expected interactive humanoid robots to be talked to by persons

被引:50
作者
Arita, A
Hiraki, K
Kanda, T
Ishiguro, H
机构
[1] Univ Tokyo, Grad Sch Arts & Sci, Dept Gen Syst Studies, Meguro Ku, Tokyo 1538902, Japan
[2] Osaka Univ, Sch Engn, Osaka, Japan
[3] ATR, Intelligent Robot & Commun Labs, Kyoto, Japan
关键词
communicative agent; human-robot interaction; humanoid robot;
D O I
10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.001
中图分类号
B84 [心理学];
学科分类号
04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
As technology advances, many human-like robots are being developed. Although these humanoid robots should be classified as objects, they share many properties with human beings. This raises the question of how infants classify them. Based on the looking-time paradigm used by [Legerstee, M., Barna, J., & DiAdamo, C., (2000). Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: understanding people and their actions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 5, 627-634.], we investigated whether 10-month-old infants expected people to talk to a humanoid robot. In a familiarization period, each infant observed an actor and an interactive robot behaving like a human, a non-interactive robot remaining stationary, and a non-interactive robot behaving like a human. In subsequent test trials, the infants were shown another actor talking to the robot and to the actor. We found that infants who had previously observed the interactive robot showed no difference in looking-time between the two types of test events. Infants in the other conditions, however, looked longer at the test event where the second experimenter talked to the robot rather than where the second experimenter talked to the person. These results suggest that infants interpret the interactive robot as a communicative agent and the non-interactive robot as an object. Our findings imply that infants categorize interactive humanoid robots as a kind of human being. (c) 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:B49 / B57
页数:9
相关论文
共 14 条
[1]   DISCRIMINATION AND IMITATION OF FACIAL EXPRESSIONS BY NEONATES [J].
FIELD, TM ;
WOODSON, R ;
GREENBERG, R ;
COHEN, D .
SCIENCE, 1982, 218 (4568) :179-181
[2]   Robovie: an interactive humanoid robot [J].
Ishiguro, H ;
Ono, T ;
Imai, M ;
Maeda, T ;
Kanda, T ;
Nakatsu, R .
INDUSTRIAL ROBOT, 2001, 28 (06) :498-503
[3]  
Johnson F, 1999, J Cutan Laser Ther, V1, P233, DOI 10.1080/14628839950516733
[4]   Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent [J].
Johnson, SC ;
Booth, A ;
O'Hearn, K .
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT, 2001, 16 (01) :637-656
[5]   PATTERNS OF 4-MONTH-OLD INFANT RESPONSES TO HIDDEN SILENT AND SOUNDING PEOPLE AND OBJECTS [J].
LEGERSTEE, M .
EARLY DEVELOPMENT & PARENTING, 1994, 3 (02) :71-80
[6]   Six-month-old infants rely on explanatory inference when relating communication to people and manipulatory actions to inanimate objects: Reply to Gergely (2001) [J].
Legerstee, M .
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 37 (05) :583-586
[7]   THE ROLE OF PERSON AND OBJECT IN ELICITING EARLY IMITATION [J].
LEGERSTEE, M .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY, 1991, 51 (03) :423-433
[8]  
Legerstee M, 1997, SOC DEV, V6, P307
[9]  
Legerstee M, 2000, DEV PSYCHOL, V36, P627, DOI 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.627
[10]   UNDERSTANDING THE INTENTIONS OF OTHERS - REENACTMENT OF INTENDED ACTS BY 18-MONTH-OLD CHILDREN [J].
MELTZOFF, AN .
DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1995, 31 (05) :838-850