Overt naming fMRI pre- and post-TMS: Two nonfluent aphasia patients, with and without improved naming post-TMS

被引:128
作者
Martin, Paula I. [1 ,2 ]
Naeser, Margaret A. [1 ,2 ]
Ho, Michael [1 ,2 ]
Doron, Karl W. [1 ,2 ]
Kurland, Jacquie [1 ,2 ]
Kaplan, Jerome [1 ,2 ]
Wang, Yunyan [1 ,2 ]
Nicholas, Marjorie [1 ,2 ]
Baker, Errol H. [1 ,2 ]
Fregni, Felipe [3 ,4 ,5 ]
Pascual-Leone, Alvaro [3 ,4 ,5 ]
机构
[1] Boston Univ, Sch Med, Dept Neurol, Harold Goodglass Boston Univ,Aphasia Res Ctr, Boston, MA 02215 USA
[2] Vet Affairs Boston Healthcare Syst, Boston, MA USA
[3] Beth Israel Deaconess Med Ctr, Berenson Allen Ctr Noninvas Brain Stimulat, Boston, MA 02215 USA
[4] Harvard Univ, Sch Med, Boston, MA USA
[5] Inst Guttmann, Barcelona, Spain
关键词
Aphasia; TMS; fMRI; Overt naming; Stroke rehabilitation; TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION; RIGHT-HEMISPHERE; POSTSTROKE APHASIA; BRAIN PLASTICITY; FUNCTIONAL MRI; HEMODYNAMIC DELAY; LANGUAGE PATHWAYS; FRONTAL-CORTEX; VERBAL FLUENCY; WORD RETRIEVAL;
D O I
10.1016/j.bandl.2009.07.007
中图分类号
R36 [病理学]; R76 [耳鼻咽喉科学];
学科分类号
100104 ; 100213 ;
摘要
Two chronic, nonfluent aphasia patients participated in overt naming fMRI scans, pre- and post-a series of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) treatments as part of a TMS study to improve naming. Each patient received 10, 1-Hz rTMS treatments to suppress a part of R pars triangularis. PI was a 'good responder' with improved naming and phrase length; P2 was a 'poor responder' without improved naming. Pre-TMS (10 years poststroke), PI had significant activation in R and L sensorimotor cortex, R IFG, and in both L and R SMA during overt naming fMRI (28% pictures named). At 3 mo. post-TMS (42% named), P1 showed continued activation in R and L sensorimotor cortex, R IFG, and in R and L SMA. At 16 mo. post-TMS (58% named), he also showed significant activation in R and L sensorimotor cortex mouth and R IFG. He now showed a significant increase in activation in the L SMA compared to pre-TMS and at 3 mo. post-TMS (p < .02; p < .05, respectively). At 16 mo. there was also greater activation in L than R SMA (p < .08). At 46 mo. post-TMS (42% named), this new LH pattern of activation continued. He improved on the Boston Naming Test from I I pictures named pre-TMS, to scores ranging from 14 to 18 pictures, post-TMS (2-43 mo. post-TMS). His longest phrase length (Cookie Theft picture) improved from three words pre-TMS, to 5-6 words post-TMS. Pre-TMS (1.5 years poststroke), P2 had significant activation in R IFG (3% pictures named). At 3 and 6 mo. post-TMS, there was no longer significant activation in R IFG, but significant activation was present in R sensorimotor cortex. On all three fMRI scans, P2 had significant activation in both the Land R SMA. There was no new, lasting perilesional LH activation across sessions for this patient. Over time, there was little or no change in his activation. His naming remained only at 1-2 pictures during all three fMRI scans. His BNT score and longest phrase length remained at one word, post-TMS. Lesion site may play a role in each patient's fMRI activation pattern and response to TMS treatment. P2, the poor responder, had an atypical frontal lesion in the L motor and premotor cortex that extended high, near brain vertex, with deep white matter lesion near L SMA, P2 also had frontal lesion in the posterior middle frontal gyrus, an area important for naming (Duffau et al., 2003); PI did not. Additionally, P2 had lesion inferior and posterior to Wernicke's area, in parts of BA 21 and 37, whereas PI did not. The fMRI data of our patient who had good response following TMS support the notion that restoration of the LH language network is linked in part, to better recovery of naming and phrase length in nonfluent aphasia. (C) 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:20 / 35
页数:16
相关论文
共 76 条
[1]   Functional magnetic resonance imaging of verbal fluency and confrontation naming using compressed image acquisition to permit overt responses [J].
Abrahams, S ;
Goldstein, LH ;
Simmons, A ;
Brammer, MJ ;
Williams, SCR ;
Giampietro, VP ;
Andrew, CM ;
Leigh, PN .
HUMAN BRAIN MAPPING, 2003, 20 (01) :29-40
[2]  
Amunts K, 1999, J COMP NEUROL, V412, P319, DOI 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990920)412:2<319::AID-CNE10>3.0.CO
[3]  
2-7
[4]   Analysis of neural mechanisms underlying verbal fluency in cytoarchitectonically defined stereotaxic space - The roles of Brodmann areas 44 and 45 [J].
Amunts, K ;
Weiss, PH ;
Mohlberg, H ;
Pieperhoff, P ;
Eickhoff, S ;
Gurd, JM ;
Marshall, JC ;
Shah, NJ ;
Fink, GR ;
Zilles, K .
NEUROIMAGE, 2004, 22 (01) :42-56
[5]  
[Anonymous], 2002, NEUROIMAGE
[6]  
[Anonymous], 2001, ASSESSMENT APHASIA R
[7]  
ANWANDER A, 2006, CEREBRAL CORTEX
[8]   Overt verbal responding during fMRI scanning: Empirical investigations of problems and potential solutions [J].
Barch, DM ;
Sabb, FW ;
Carter, CS ;
Braver, TS ;
Noll, DC ;
Cohen, JD .
NEUROIMAGE, 1999, 10 (06) :642-657
[9]  
BARLOW T, 1877, BRIT MED J, V103
[10]   THE ROLE OF THE RIGHT-HEMISPHERE IN RECOVERY FROM APHASIA - 2 CASE-STUDIES [J].
BASSO, A ;
GARDELLI, M ;
GRASSI, MP ;
MARIOTTI, M .
CORTEX, 1989, 25 (04) :555-566