Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to motivated audiences about science topics

被引:327
作者
Fiske, Susan T. [1 ]
Dupree, Cydney [1 ]
机构
[1] Princeton Univ, Princeton, NJ 08544 USA
关键词
public images; scientist stereotypes; STEREOTYPE CONTENT MODEL; CLIMATE-CHANGE; UNCERTAINTY; COMPETITION; COMPETENCE; ATTITUDES; RESPONSES; COGNITION; BEHAVIOR; WARMTH;
D O I
10.1073/pnas.1317505111
中图分类号
O [数理科学和化学]; P [天文学、地球科学]; Q [生物科学]; N [自然科学总论];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
Expertise is a prerequisite for communicator credibility, entailing the knowledge and ability to be accurate. Trust also is essential to communicator credibility. Audiences view trustworthiness as the motivation to be truthful. Identifying whom to trust follows systematic principles. People decide quickly another's apparent intent: Who is friend or foe, on their side or not, or a cooperator or competitor. Those seemingly on their side are deemed warm (friendly, trustworthy). People then decide whether the other is competent to enact those intents. Perception of scientists, like other social perceptions, involves inferring both their apparent intent (warmth) and capability (competence). To illustrate, we polled adults online about typical American jobs, rated as American society views them, on warmth and competence dimensions, as well as relevant emotions. Ambivalently perceived high-competence but low-warmth, "envied" professions included lawyers, chief executive officers, engineers, accountants, scientists, and researchers. Being seen as competent but cold might not seem problematic until one recalls that communicator credibility requires not just status and expertise but also trustworthiness (warmth). Other research indicates the risk from being enviable. Turning to a case study of scientific communication, another online sample of adults described public attitudes toward climate scientists specifically. Although distrust is low, the apparent motive to gain research money is distrusted. The literature on climate science communicators agrees that the public trusts impartiality, not persuasive agendas. Overall, communicator credibility needs to address both expertise and trustworthiness. Scientists have earned audiences' respect, but not necessarily their trust. Discussing, teaching, and sharing information can earn trust to show scientists' trustworthy intentions.
引用
收藏
页码:13593 / 13597
页数:5
相关论文
共 44 条
[1]  
Allen D., 2004, Talking to Strangers: Anxieties of Citizenship Since Brown v. Board of Education
[2]  
Aristotle, 1959, ARS RHETORICA
[3]   THE NEED TO BELONG - DESIRE FOR INTERPERSONAL ATTACHMENTS AS A FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN-MOTIVATION [J].
BAUMEISTER, RF ;
LEARY, MR .
PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 1995, 117 (03) :497-529
[4]   Stereotyping by Omission: Eliminate the Negative, Accentuate the Positive [J].
Bergsieker, Hilary B. ;
Leslie, Lisa M. ;
Constantine, Vanessa S. ;
Fiske, Susan T. .
JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2012, 102 (06) :1214-1238
[5]   Trust in scientific experts on obesity: Implications for awareness and behavior change [J].
Bleich, Sara ;
Blendon, Robert ;
Adams, Alyce .
OBESITY, 2007, 15 (08) :2145-2156
[6]  
Brewer Marilynn., 1998, HDB SOCIAL PSYCHOL, P554, DOI DOI 10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1414
[7]   Stereotypes and Schadenfreude: Affective and Physiological Markers of Pleasure at Outgroup Misfortunes [J].
Cikara, Mina ;
Fiske, Susan T. .
SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PERSONALITY SCIENCE, 2012, 3 (01) :63-71
[8]   Us Versus Them: Social Identity Shapes Neural Responses to Intergroup Competition and Harm [J].
Cikara, Mina ;
Botvinick, Matthew M. ;
Fiske, Susan T. .
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2011, 22 (03) :306-313
[9]   When do subgroup parts add up to the stereotypic whole? Mixed stereotype content for gay male subgroups explains overall ratings [J].
Clausell, E .
SOCIAL COGNITION, 2005, 23 (02) :161-181
[10]   Evaluating Science Arguments: Evidence, Uncertainty, and Argument Strength [J].
Corner, Adam ;
Hahn, Ulrike .
JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-APPLIED, 2009, 15 (03) :199-212