Evolving the selfish herd: emergence of distinct aggregating strategies in an individual-based model

被引:124
作者
Wood, Andrew J.
Ackland, Graeme J.
机构
[1] Univ Edinburgh, SUPA, Sch Phys, Edinburgh EH9 3JZ, Midlothian, Scotland
[2] Univ York, Dept Biol, YCCSA, York YO10 5YW, N Yorkshire, England
基金
英国工程与自然科学研究理事会;
关键词
flocking; evolution; genetic algorithm; predation; foraging; Nash equilibrium; FISH SCHOOLS; BEHAVIOR; PREDATION; FLOCKING; TRANSITION; SIMULATION; FORAGERS; MOVEMENT; DYNAMICS; EVASION;
D O I
10.1098/rspb.2007.0306
中图分类号
Q [生物科学];
学科分类号
07 ; 0710 ; 09 ;
摘要
From zebra to starlings, herring and even tadpoles, many creatures move in an organized group. The emergent behaviour arises from simple underlying movement rules, but the evolutionary pressure which favours these rules has not been conclusively identified. Various explanations exist for the advantage to the individual of group formation: reduction of predation risk; increased foraging efficiency or reproductive success. Here, we adopt an individual-based model for group formation and subject it to simulated predation and foraging; the haploid individuals evolve via a genetic algorithm based on their relative success under such pressure. Our work suggests that flock or herd formation is likely to be driven by predator avoidance. Individual fitness in the model is strongly dependent on the presence of other phenotypes, such that two distinct types of evolved group can be produced by the same predation or foraging conditions, each stable against individual mutation. We draw analogies with multiple Nash equilibria theory of iterated games to explain and categorize these behaviours. Our model is sufficient to capture the complex behaviour of dynamic collective groups, yet is flexible enough to manifest evolutionary behaviour.
引用
收藏
页码:1637 / 1642
页数:6
相关论文
共 29 条
[1]  
AOKI I, 1982, B JPN SOC SCI FISH, V48, P1081
[2]   Experimental evidences of a structural and dynamical transition in fish school [J].
Becco, C ;
Vandewalle, N ;
Delcourt, J ;
Poncin, P .
PHYSICA A-STATISTICAL MECHANICS AND ITS APPLICATIONS, 2006, 367 (487-493) :487-493
[3]   FLOCK SIZE, FOOD DISPERSION, AND THE FEEDING-BEHAVIOR OF CROSSBILLS [J].
BENKMAN, CW .
BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY AND SOCIOBIOLOGY, 1988, 23 (03) :167-175
[4]  
CODY M L, 1971, Theoretical Population Biology, V2, P142, DOI 10.1016/0040-5809(71)90012-8
[5]   Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups [J].
Couzin, ID ;
Krause, J ;
James, R ;
Ruxton, GD ;
Franks, NR .
JOURNAL OF THEORETICAL BIOLOGY, 2002, 218 (01) :1-11
[6]   FLOCKING IS AN EFFECTIVE ANTI-PREDATION STRATEGY IN REDSHANKS, TRINGA-TETANUS [J].
CRESSWELL, W .
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1994, 47 (02) :433-442
[7]   PREDATOR EVASION IN A FISH SCHOOL - TEST OF A MODEL FOR THE FOUNTAIN EFFECT [J].
HALL, SJ ;
WARDLE, CS ;
MACLENNAN, DN .
MARINE BIOLOGY, 1986, 91 (01) :143-148
[8]   GEOMETRY FOR SELFISH HERD [J].
HAMILTON, WD .
JOURNAL OF THEORETICAL BIOLOGY, 1971, 31 (02) :295-+
[9]   Modelling the many-wrongs principle: The navigational advantages of aggregation in nomadic foragers [J].
Hancock, Penelope A. ;
Milner-Gulland, E. J. ;
Keeling, Matthew J. .
JOURNAL OF THEORETICAL BIOLOGY, 2006, 240 (02) :302-310
[10]   Predator avoidance as a function of flocking in the sexually dichromatic Hawaii akepa [J].
Hart, PJ ;
Freed, LA .
JOURNAL OF ETHOLOGY, 2005, 23 (01) :29-33