Stability and Change in Intelligence From Age 11 to Ages 70, 79, and 87: The Lothian Birth Cohorts of 1921 and 1936

被引:119
作者
Gow, Alan J. [1 ]
Johnson, Wendy [1 ]
Pattie, Alison
Brett, Caroline E.
Roberts, Beverly [1 ]
Starr, John M. [2 ]
Deary, Ian J. [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Edinburgh, Dept Psychol, Ctr Cognit Ageing & Cognit Epidemiol, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, Midlothian, Scotland
[2] Univ Edinburgh, Geriatr Med Unit, Ctr Cognit Ageing & Cognit Epidemiol, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, Midlothian, Scotland
基金
英国生物技术与生命科学研究理事会; 英国医学研究理事会;
关键词
cognitive ability; stability; change; cognitive aging; CHILDHOOD INTELLIGENCE; COGNITIVE DECLINE; LIFE-STYLE; ABILITY; MIDLIFE; KINDER; LOSE; IQ;
D O I
10.1037/a0021072
中图分类号
R4 [临床医学]; R592 [老年病学];
学科分类号
1002 ; 100203 ; 100602 ;
摘要
Investigating the predictors of age-related cognitive change is a research priority. However, it is first necessary to discover the long-term stability of measures of cognitive ability because prior cognitive ability level might contribute to the amount of cognitive change experienced within old age. These two issues were examined in the Lothian Birth Cohorts of 1921 and 1936. Cognitive ability data were available from age 11 years when the participants completed the Moray House Test No. 12 (MHT). The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 (LBC1936) completed the MHT a second time at age 70. The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 (LBC1921) completed the MHT at ages 79 and 87. We examined cognitive stability and change from childhood to old age in both cohorts, and within old age in the LBC1921. Raw stability coefficients for the MHT from 11-70, 11-79, and 11-87 years were .67, .66, and .51, respectively; and larger when corrected for range restriction in the samples. Therefore, minimum estimates of the variance in later-life MHT accounted for by childhood performance on the same test ranged from 26-44%. This study also examined, in the LBC1921, whether MHT score at age 11 influenced the amount of change in MHT between ages 79 and 87. It did not. Higher intelligence from early life was apparently protective of intelligence in old age due to the stability of cognitive function across the lifespan, rather than because it slowed the decline experienced in later life.
引用
收藏
页码:232 / 240
页数:9
相关论文
共 40 条
[31]   Cognitive ability in childhood and cognitive decline in mid-life: longitudinal birth cohort study [J].
Richards, M ;
Shipley, B ;
Fuhrer, R ;
Wadsworth, MEJ .
BMJ-BRITISH MEDICAL JOURNAL, 2004, 328 (7439) :552-554B
[32]   Early life linguistic ability, late life cognitive function, and neuropathology: findings from the Nun Study [J].
Riley, KP ;
Snowdon, DA ;
Desrosiers, MF ;
Markesbery, WR .
NEUROBIOLOGY OF AGING, 2005, 26 (03) :341-347
[33]   Mental Exercise and Mental Aging Evaluating the Validity of the "Use It or Lose It" Hypothesis [J].
Salthouse, Timothy A. .
PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE, 2006, 1 (01) :68-87
[34]   Cognitive reserve and lifestyle [J].
Scarmeas, N ;
Stern, Y .
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 25 (05) :625-633
[35]   The Seattle Longitudinal Study: Relationship between personality and cognition [J].
Schaie, KW ;
Willis, SL ;
Caskie, GIL .
AGING NEUROPSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITION, 2004, 11 (2-3) :304-324
[36]  
Scottish Council for Research in Education, 1958, ED OTH ASP 1947 SCOT
[37]  
*UN SECR DEP EC SO, 2005, WORLD POP PROSP 2004
[38]   Linking early development with ageing: towards a new research agenda [J].
Westendorp, RGJ ;
Wimmer, C .
MECHANISMS OF AGEING AND DEVELOPMENT, 2005, 126 (03) :419-420
[39]   Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76 [J].
Whalley, LJ ;
Deary, IJ .
BRITISH MEDICAL JOURNAL, 2001, 322 (7290) :819-822
[40]  
Wiberg M., 2009, Practical Assessment, Research Evaluation, V14